首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2186篇
  免费   110篇
各国政治   143篇
工人农民   106篇
世界政治   216篇
外交国际关系   119篇
法律   1038篇
中国政治   12篇
政治理论   643篇
综合类   19篇
  2023年   11篇
  2022年   5篇
  2021年   19篇
  2020年   48篇
  2019年   56篇
  2018年   71篇
  2017年   92篇
  2016年   73篇
  2015年   58篇
  2014年   83篇
  2013年   335篇
  2012年   74篇
  2011年   100篇
  2010年   61篇
  2009年   80篇
  2008年   64篇
  2007年   85篇
  2006年   87篇
  2005年   86篇
  2004年   75篇
  2003年   89篇
  2002年   82篇
  2001年   44篇
  2000年   46篇
  1999年   38篇
  1998年   39篇
  1997年   39篇
  1996年   30篇
  1995年   35篇
  1994年   31篇
  1993年   21篇
  1992年   21篇
  1991年   16篇
  1990年   24篇
  1989年   14篇
  1988年   25篇
  1987年   16篇
  1986年   9篇
  1985年   18篇
  1984年   16篇
  1983年   12篇
  1982年   7篇
  1981年   9篇
  1980年   10篇
  1979年   5篇
  1978年   5篇
  1976年   7篇
  1973年   4篇
  1970年   3篇
  1967年   3篇
排序方式: 共有2296条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
201.
针对俄罗斯归属于欧洲还是亚洲的问题,传统上公认的解说有:俄罗斯是一个欧洲国家;俄罗斯不同于亚洲;俄罗斯是一个欧亚国家。这些观点各有道理,但需细化。下面几个论题将有助于对俄罗斯属性的进一步认识:1)俄罗斯不仅是一个欧洲国家,而且是一个欧洲帝国。2)激进的欧亚主义者不反对俄罗斯的欧洲性,但反对认为俄罗斯是一个帝国。3)欧亚主义者宣称俄罗斯—欧亚不是亚洲的一个部分。欧亚主义者将欧亚理解为与欧洲或者亚洲完全不同的文明。4)欧亚主义者重新将俄罗斯—欧亚视为一个潜在的殖民地,而俄罗斯的使命是反殖民的领导者。  相似文献   
202.
This column provides a concise alerting service of important national developments in key European countries. Part of its purpose is to compliment the Journal's feature articles and Briefing Notes by keeping readers abreast of what is currently happening “on the ground” at a national level in implementing EU level legislation and international conventions and treaties. Where an item of European National News is of particular significance, CLSR may also cover it in more detail in the current or a subsequent edition.  相似文献   
203.
204.
205.
206.
207.
Intentionalism is the view that an utterance (understood broadly as a spoken phrase, a written text, or even a gesture) means what its utterer intends to mean by it. In the philosophy of language, Grice's version of intentionalism rules the day, but intentionalism in statutory interpretation has well-known enemies. Antonin Scalia argues that the search for the intentions of actual legislators destroys the publicity of law and leads judges into fact-finding rather than rule-application. The author argues for a sophisticated version of intentionalism according to which the utterer of law is not an empirical individual or group of individuals but a hypothetical sovereign. The meaning of a legal text is whatever an ideally rational and reasonable communicator would intend to mean by it. By divorcing the meaning of law from the actual intentions of sometimes-conflicted and insincere legislators, this theory preserves the publicity of law and helps avoid fact-finding. Furthermore, it does justice to canons of construction (e.g., ejusdem generis, noscitur a sociis), which seem to aim at what an ideally rational and reasonable lawgiver would mean by a given legal text. Finally, hypothetical intentionalism accords with actual judicial practice, at least in the United States Supreme Court, where justices often seem to tacitly assume that the utterer of law always expresses her meaning in the best possible way.  相似文献   
208.
Purpose. In police interviews children may be asked the same question many times. We investigated how the number of repetitions and the interval between those repetitions affected the accuracy and consistency of children's responses. Methods. 156 children aged 4–9 years watched a staged event and were interviewed individually 1 week later. Children were asked eight open‐ended questions, which were each repeated a further four times (making a total of forty questions). Half these open‐ended questions could be answered from information in the event, and half were unanswerable (so children should have said ‘don't know’ in response to these questions). The questions were repeated in gist form. The interval between an initial question and its repetitions was varied by use of other questions and twenty non‐repeated filler questions. The intervals between repetitions were immediate repetition, repetition after a delay of three intervening questions, after a delay of six intervening questions, and after ten or more intervening questions. Results. Over a quarter of children's responses to repeated questions changed, usually resulting in a decline in accuracy, particularly after the first repetition. Subsequently, the number of repetitions and delay interval had little effect on responses to answerable questions although accuracy to unanswerable questions continued to decline. Conclusions. Question repetition had a negative affect on children's consistency and accuracy. For unanswerable questions in particular, the more often a question was repeated the more likely children were to invent a response.  相似文献   
209.
210.
BIRDS OF PRAYER     
  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号