首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   75315篇
  免费   3322篇
各国政治   4555篇
工人农民   3050篇
世界政治   6495篇
外交国际关系   3952篇
法律   37453篇
中国共产党   44篇
中国政治   805篇
政治理论   21255篇
综合类   1028篇
  2021年   434篇
  2020年   1195篇
  2019年   1547篇
  2018年   1781篇
  2017年   2085篇
  2016年   2280篇
  2015年   1880篇
  2014年   2134篇
  2013年   10689篇
  2012年   1780篇
  2011年   1951篇
  2010年   1942篇
  2009年   2175篇
  2008年   1995篇
  2007年   1978篇
  2006年   2109篇
  2005年   2008篇
  2004年   1888篇
  2003年   1673篇
  2002年   1710篇
  2001年   1991篇
  2000年   1755篇
  1999年   1526篇
  1998年   1209篇
  1997年   1052篇
  1996年   1019篇
  1995年   975篇
  1994年   1016篇
  1993年   1028篇
  1992年   1123篇
  1991年   1210篇
  1990年   1110篇
  1989年   1178篇
  1988年   1171篇
  1987年   1171篇
  1986年   1152篇
  1985年   1208篇
  1984年   1054篇
  1983年   1100篇
  1982年   941篇
  1981年   902篇
  1980年   712篇
  1979年   794篇
  1978年   649篇
  1977年   577篇
  1976年   518篇
  1975年   445篇
  1974年   474篇
  1973年   485篇
  1972年   407篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
311.
A minisatellite probe, MZ 1.3, detecting hypervariable fragment patterns was isolated from a human genomic library. A repetitive sequence of 27 bp length was identified which is contained in the probe approx. 40 times. The MZ 1.3 repeat shows variable homology of 53-73% to the repetitive sequence of the protein III gene of the bacteriophage M13 genome. Polymorphic restriction fragment patterns were found with MZ 1.3 using the enzymes Hinf I, BstN I, Hae III, Mbo I, PstI/Pvu II, and Rsa I. An average of 18 polymorphic fragments was observed using Hinf I as enzyme. The band sharing frequency after Hinf I digestion among unrelated individuals was determined to be 23.8 +/- 7.2%. An example for the application of MZ 1.3 to paternity testing in an incest case is given. The probe can be used with radioactive or non-radioactive detection systems. An approach is presented to compare polymorphic fragment patterns from individuals obtained by independent gel runs on the basis of relative band positions (RBP) and calculated in a computerized analysis.  相似文献   
312.
313.
The Supreme Court early took note of extralegal, “social science” materials in Muller v. Oregon (1908), and a half-century later made specific reference to social science authorities in the famous footnote 11 of Brown v. Board of Education (1954). Since Brown, much has been written about the Supreme Court's use of social science research evidence, but there has been little systematic study of that use. Those writing on the subject commonly focus on areas of law such as jury size, where social science has been used, and have generally assumed that social science information has been utilized in Supreme Court decisions with increasing regularity. Surprisingly little is known, however, about either the justices' baseline use of social science authorities, or many other aspects of their uses of social science information. The focus here is on the citation of social science research evidence in a sample of 240 criminal cases decided during the 30 years between the Supreme Court's 1958 and 1987 Terms. The resulting portrait contributes to a fuller understanding of the justices' use of social science materials, and may ultimately help promote more effective utilization of social science research evidence in Supreme Court decisions.  相似文献   
314.
315.
316.
317.
318.
319.
The extent to which local life circumstances influence criminal activity has been the focus of much theoretical debate. Although empirical research has been initiated, it remains limited. Herein, we use data on 524 serious offenders from the California Youth Authority for a seven‐year post‐parole period to examine the relationship between changes in local life circumstances and criminal activity. We extend previous research by employing a statistical model that accounts for the joint distribution of violent and nonviolent crime during the late teens and twenties in order to present information on patterns of criminal activity during a newly recognized developmental period of the life course, “emerging adulthood.”  相似文献   
320.
On the basis of the uncertainty management model, we argue here that when people are uncertain about an organization's trustworthiness, they may resolve the question how they should react toward the organization by relying on their perceptions of the organization's procedures. As a consequence, we predicted that the reactions of parents whose child was in a day care center would be strongly influenced by their perceptions of the procedures used by the organization that was responsible for their children's day care when the parents would be uncertain about the organization's trustworthiness. However, when parents would be certain that the organization could be trusted they would be less in need of procedural information, yielding less strong effects of perceived procedure on parents' reactions. The findings of a survey study corroborate this line of reasoning. In the discussion it is argued that these findings suggest that people especially rely on their perceptions of procedures when they are uncertain about important aspects of their lives, such as the trustworthiness of organizations that are responsible for their children's day care.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号