排序方式: 共有45条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
21.
Do people realize the danger of asking misinformed children yes–no questions? Study 1 confirmed that disclosures children made during free recall in an earlier suggestibility study were more accurate than disclosures following “yes” responses to yes–no questions, which in turn were more accurate than disclosures following “no” responses. In Studies 2 and 3, college students watched interviews of children and judged the veracity of these three disclosure patterns. Participants generally believed false reports representing the first two patterns, although watching expert testimony that included a videotaped example of a false report reduced trust in prompted disclosures. Results document the need to inform forensic decision-makers about the circumstances associated with erroneous responses to yes–no questions. 相似文献
22.
Students watched a theft video, attempted an identification from a thief-present or thief-absent lineup under unbiased or
biased instructions, and rated identification confidence. In Experiment 1, the participants received (bogus) positive, negative,
or no pre-identification feedback about a recall test. Biased instructions and positive feedback increased confidence and
ratings of eyewitnessing conditions. In Experiment 2, biased instructions increased confidence unless the thief was absent
and lineup members were similar, where they decreased confidence. According to the cue-belief model, biased instructions send
a positive accuracy cue regarding the most familiar-looking lineup member. If none stands out, instructions conflict with
an inclination to reject the lineup. Feedback may create a belief about memory quality that is a cue regarding likely recognition
accuracy.
相似文献
Michael R. LeippeEmail: |
23.
Pairs (N = 234) of witnesses and lineup administrators completed an identification task in which administrator knowledge, lineup presentation,
instruction bias, and target presence were manipulated. Administrator knowledge had the greatest effect on identifications
of the suspect for simultaneous photospreads paired with biased instructions, with single-blind administrations increasing
identifications of the suspect. When biased instructions were given, single-blind administrations produced fewer foil identifications
than double-blind administrations. Administrators exhibited a greater proportion of biasing behaviors during single-blind
administrations than during double-blind administrations. The diagnosticity of identifications of the suspect in double-blind
administrations was double their diagnosticity in single-blind administrations. These results suggest that when biasing factors
are present to increase a witness’s propensity to guess, single-blind administrator behavior influences witnesses to identify
the suspect. 相似文献
24.
A. Daniel Yarmey 《心理学、犯罪与法律》2013,19(1):53-68
Some 590 men and women were tested in public places for interrogative recall and photo identification of a young woman to whom they had spoken for approximately 15 seconds, either 2 minutes earlier or 4 hours earlier. The target was seen originally either with or without a baseball cap and dark sunglasses. Witnesses were either prepared or not prepared at the time of the encounter for a subsequent memory test. Half of the witnesses were given imagery retrieval instructions or standard retrieval instructions prior to the two memory tests. A separate group of 379 introductory psychology students attempted to predict the performance of the eyewitnesses. Witness preparation was of more importance for recall of clothing characteristics than for physical characteristics. Witness preparation, target disguise, retention interval, gender of witnesses, and retrieval instructions had no significant main effects on identification. Forty-nine per cent of the witnesses given the target-present lineup correctly identified the target, and 62% correctly rejected the target-absent lineup. Student's beliefs in the accuracy of recall and identification were not consistent with eyewitnesses’ performance. 相似文献
25.
Abstract Preschoolers’ abilities to recognize former caregivers were examined. Participants were 35 children from three preschool classes. Caregivers volunteered in the classes over a 7-week period. At an initial assessment, children were asked to select former caregivers from a line-up of five photographs, and to rank-order the caregivers by personal preference. Recognition was assessed again after 3 months. Although older toddlers performed at chance levels, clear age-related increases in recognition performance were observed, with older preschoolers recognizing 86% of the volunteers at time 2. Younger children responded less consistently over time than older children. Preference for individual caregivers affected recognition performance among younger, but not older, preschoolers. Implications for children's eyewitness testimony are considered. 相似文献
26.
Lindsay, Lea, and Fulford (J Appl Psychol 76:741–745, 1991) found that false identifications increased, correct rejections decreased, and correct identifications minimally increased
when witnesses were allowed a second viewing of a sequential lineup. We conducted a similar study in which we allowed mock
witnesses a second viewing of a sequential lineup. Results indicated that participants who were allowed a second viewing made
mostly incorrect identifications. Participants correctly identified the perpetrator more often when they were not told in
advance that they would have a second viewing than when they were told. Additionally, those participants who were told that
they would be allowed a second viewing incorrectly rejected the lineup more often than those who were not told. The findings
yielded conclusions similar to those of Lindsay et al. (J Appl Psychol 76:741–745, 1991) suggesting that witnesses should not be allowed to view the lineup a second time. The results also suggest that, if a repeated
lineup is used, witnesses should not be informed of that in advance. 相似文献
27.
We examined the hypothesis that a pointed knife attracts particular attention because of its pointed shape. Using a visual
search task method, we asked 33 participants to search 3 × 3 matrices containing outlines of knives with pointed blades such
as chef’s knives and knives with rectangular blades such as Chinese chopping knives and to determine whether the matrices
contained identical or non-identical knives. The participants found a pointed knife within a background of knives with rectangular
blades more quickly than under the reverse condition. This result indicates that the pointed shape of a knife has an attribute
that captures human attention and suggests that the visual features of a weapon affect eyewitness perception. This research
would benefit forensic investigations in countries such as Japan, where gun ownership is banned and perpetrators use knives
more often than firearms. 相似文献
28.
Neuschatz JS Lawson DS Fairless AH Powers RA Neuschatz JS Goodsell CA Toglia MP 《Law and human behavior》2007,31(3):231-247
Three studies examined procedures for reducing the post-identification feedback effect. After viewing a video event, participants were then asked to identify a suspect from a target-absent photo lineup. After making their identification, some participants were given information suggesting that their identification was correct, while others were given no information about the accuracy of their identification. Some participants who received confirming feedback were also given reasons to entertain suspicion regarding the motives of the lineup administrator, either immediately (Experiment 1) or after a one-week retention interval (Experiment 2). Suspicious perceivers failed to demonstrate the confidence inflation effects typically associated with confirming post-identification feedback. In Experiment 3, the confidence prophylactic effect was tested both immediately and after a one-week retention interval. The effect of confidence prophylactic varied with retention interval such that it eliminated the effects of post-identification feedback immediately but not after a retention interval. However, the suspicion manipulation eliminated the post-identification feedback effects at both time intervals. Both theoretical and practical implications are discussed. 相似文献
29.
《Science & justice》2023,63(4):537-541
Environmental context reinstatement has a particular value for recall of information in forensic interviews. Since odors are valuable memory cues and can act as memory triggers, in our preliminary study we explored whether odor exposure can help people recall details of a crime scene. The study comprised 58 women and 15 men aged 22–35 who immersed in a carefully controlled environment closely resembling an actual crime setting, i.e., a virtual reality crime. Participants were exposed to an odor at encoding, recall, both or neither of these instances, yielding a total of 4 experimental groups that further completed a memory recall task. The crime scene content recall was tested in a free recall and a forced-response test immediately after seeing the crime scene and one month later. We found no significant effects of odor exposure on the free or the cued recall of the crime scene. The memory scores correlated neither with the self-assessed olfactory/visual sensitivity of the subjects, nor with the perceived odor pleasantness. These preliminary findings suggest that introduction of a vanilla odor while encoding and recalling a crime scene does not aid witness recall accuracy. 相似文献
30.
Jurors are heavily swayed by confident eyewitnesses. Are they also influenced by how eyewitnesses justify their level of confidence? Here we document a counter-intuitive effect: when eyewitnesses identified a suspect from a lineup with absolute certainty (‘I am completely confident’) and justified their confidence by referring to a visible feature of the accused (‘I remember his nose’), participants judged the suspect as less likely to be guilty than when eyewitnesses identified a suspect with absolute certainty but offered an unobservable justification (‘I would never forget him’) or no justification at all. Moreover, people perceive an eyewitness’s identification as nearly 25% less accurate when the eyewitness has provided a featural justification than an unobservable justification or simply no justification. Even when an eyewitness’s level of confidence is clear because s/he has expressed it numerically (e.g. ‘I am 100% certain’) participants perceive eyewitnesses as not credible (i.e. inaccurate) when the eyewitness has provided a featural justification. However, the effect of featural justifications – relative to a confidence statement only – is maximal when there is an accompanying lineup of faces, moderate when there is a single face and minimal when there is no face at all. The results support our Perceived-Diagnosticity account. 相似文献